Difference between revisions of "2018:Audio Tempo Estimation"
(→Collections) |
(→Perceptual tempo data collection) |
||
Line 74: | Line 74: | ||
The following procedure is described in more detail in McKinney and Moelants (2004) and Moelants and McKinney (2004). Listeners were asked to tap to the beat of a series of musical excerpts. Responses were collected and their perceived tempo was calculated. For each excerpt, a distribution of perceived tempo was generated. A relatively simple form of perceived tempo was proposed for this contest: The two highest peaks in the perceived tempo distribution for each excerpt were taken, along with their respective heights (normalized to sum to 1.0) as the two tempo candidates for that particular excerpt. The height of a peak in the distribution is assumed to represent the perceptual salience of that tempo. | The following procedure is described in more detail in McKinney and Moelants (2004) and Moelants and McKinney (2004). Listeners were asked to tap to the beat of a series of musical excerpts. Responses were collected and their perceived tempo was calculated. For each excerpt, a distribution of perceived tempo was generated. A relatively simple form of perceived tempo was proposed for this contest: The two highest peaks in the perceived tempo distribution for each excerpt were taken, along with their respective heights (normalized to sum to 1.0) as the two tempo candidates for that particular excerpt. The height of a peak in the distribution is assumed to represent the perceptual salience of that tempo. | ||
+ | |||
+ | Perceptual tempo data collection for the GiantSteps dataset (Knees et al. 2015) was conducted in an online tapping experiment described in detail in Schreiber und Müller (2018). Just like the original McKinney/Moelants dataset, its annotations feature two tempi and a relative salience value. The dataset is publicly available for inspection, but must not be used for training. | ||
==== References ==== | ==== References ==== | ||
* McKinney, M.F. and Moelants, D. (2004), Deviations from the resonance theory of tempo induction, Conference on Interdisciplinary Musicology, Graz. URL: http://www-gewi.uni-graz.at/staff/parncutt/cim04/CIM04_paper_pdf/McKinney_Moelants_CIM04_proceedings_t.pdf | * McKinney, M.F. and Moelants, D. (2004), Deviations from the resonance theory of tempo induction, Conference on Interdisciplinary Musicology, Graz. URL: http://www-gewi.uni-graz.at/staff/parncutt/cim04/CIM04_paper_pdf/McKinney_Moelants_CIM04_proceedings_t.pdf | ||
− | * Moelants, D. and McKinney, M.F. (2004), Tempo perception and musical content: What makes a piece slow, fast, or temporally ambiguous? International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition, Evanston, IL. URL: http://icmpc8.umn.edu/proceedings/ICMPC8/PDF/AUTHOR/MP040237.PDF | + | * Moelants, D. and McKinney, M.F. (2004), Tempo perception and musical content: What makes a piece slow, fast, or temporally ambiguous? International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition, Evanston, IL. URL: http://icmpc8.umn.edu/proceedings/ICMPC8/PDF/AUTHOR/MP040237.PDF |
+ | *Schreiber, H. and Müller, M. (2018), A Crowdsourced Experiment for Tempo Estimation of Electronic Dance Music. In Proceedings of the 19th International Society for Music Information Retrieval Conference (ISMIR), Paris, France, Sept. 2018. URL: http://www.tagtraum.com/download/2018_schreiber_tempo_giantsteps.pdf | ||
+ | *Knees, P. et al. (2015), Two data sets for tempo estimation and key detection in electronic dance music annotated from user corrections. In Proceedings of the 16th International Society for Music Information Retrieval Conference (ISMIR), Málaga, Spain, October 2015. URL: http://www.mtg.upf.edu/system/files/publications/246_Paper.pdf | ||
=== Evaluation of tempo extraction algorithms === | === Evaluation of tempo extraction algorithms === |
Revision as of 18:52, 2 August 2018
Contents
Description
This task compares current methods for the extraction of tempo from musical audio. We distinguish between notated tempo and perceptual tempo and will test for the extraction of perceptual tempo.
We differentiate between notated tempo and perceived tempo. If you have the notated tempo (e.g., from the score) it is straightforward attach a tempo annotation to an excerpt and run a contest for algorithms to predict the notated tempo. For excerpts for which we have no "official" tempo annotation, we can also annotate the *perceived* tempo. This is not a straightforward task and needs to be done carefully. If you ask a group of listeners (including skilled musicians) to annotate the tempo of music excerpts, they can give you different answers (they tap at different metrical levels) if they are unfamiliar with the piece. For some excerpts the perceived pulse or tempo is less ambiguous and everyone taps at the same metrical level, but for other excerpts the tempo can be quite ambiguous and you get a complete split across listeners.
The annotation of perceptual tempo can take several forms: a probability density function as a function of tempo; a series of tempos, ranked by their respective perceptual salience; etc. These measures of perceptual tempo can be used as a ground truth on which to test algorithms for tempo extraction. The dominant perceived tempo is sometimes the same as the notated tempo but not always. A piece of music can "feel" faster or slower than it's notated tempo in that the dominant perceived pulse can be a metrical level higher or lower than the notated tempo.
There are several reasons to examine the perceptual tempo, either in place of or in addition to the notated tempo. For many applications of automatic tempo extractors, the perceived tempo of the music is more relevant than the notated tempo. An automatic playlist generator or music navigator, for instance, might allow listeners to select or filter music by its (automatically extracted) tempo. In this case, the "feel", or perceptual tempo may be more relevant than the notated tempo. An automatic DJ apparatus might also perform better with a representation of perceived tempo rather than notated tempo.
A more pragmatic reason for using perceptual tempo rather than notated tempo as a ground truth for our contest is that we simply do not have the notated tempo of our test set. If we notate it by having a panel of expert listeners tap along and label the excerpts, we are by default dealing with the perceived tempo. The handling of this data as ground truth must be done with care.
Data
Collections
This year algorithm will be evaluated on two datasets:
- MIREX 2006 Tempo dataset collected by Martin F. McKinney (Philips) and Dirk Moelants (IPEM, Ghent University). Composed of 160 30-second clips in WAV format with annotated tempos.
- GiantSteps tempo dataset (Knees et al. 2015), using the perceptual annotations by Schreiber and Müller (2018). This dataset exclusively features electronic dance music (EDM) and is publicly available. If you are interested in a fair and unbiased evaluation, you must not use the dataset for training or validation, but only for informational purposes.
Audio Formats
The data are monophonic sound files, with the associated onset times and data about the annotation robustness.
- CD-quality (PCM, 16-bit, 44100 Hz)
- single channel (mono)
- 30 second clips
Submission Format
Submissions to this task will have to conform to a specified format detailed below. Submissions should be packaged and contain at least two files: The algorithm itself and a README containing contact information and detailing, in full, the use of the algorithm.
Input data
Individual audio files in WAV format (30-second clips drawn from the 140 unseen tracks in the dataset). The audio recordings were selected to provide a stable tempo value, a wide distribution of tempi values, and a large variety of instrumentation and musical styles. About 20% of the files contain non-binary meters, and a small number of examples contain changing meters.
Output Data
Submitted programs should output two tempi (a slower tempo, T1, and a faster tempo, T2) as well as the strength of T1 relative to T2 (0-1). The relative strength ST2 (not output) is simply 1 - ST1. The tempo estimates from each algorithm should be written to a text file in the following format:
T1<tab>T2<tab>ST1
E.g.
60 180 0.7
Algorithm Calling Format
The submitted algorithm must take as arguments a SINGLE .wav file to perform the tempo estimation detection on as well as the full output path and filename of the output file. The ability to specify the output path and file name is essential. Denoting the input .wav file path and name as %input and the output file path and name as %output, a program called foobar could be called from the command-line as follows:
foobar %input %output
or
foobar -i %input -o %output
Moreover, if your submission takes additional parameters, foobar could be called like:
foobar .1 %input %output foobar -param1 .1 -i %input -o %output
If your submission is in MATLAB, it should be submitted as a function. Once again, the function must contain String inputs for the full path and names of the input and output files. Parameters could also be specified as input arguments of the function. For example:
foobar('%input','%output') foobar(.1,'%input','%output')
README File
A README file accompanying each submission should contain explicit instructions on how to to run the program (as well as contact information, etc.). In particular, each command line to run should be specified, using %input for the input sound file and %output for the resulting text file.
Evaluation Procedures
This section focuses on the mechanics of the method while we discuss the data (music excerpts and perceptual data) in the next section. There are two general steps to the method: 1) collection of perceptual tempo annotations; and 2) evaluation of tempo extraction algorithms.
Perceptual tempo data collection
The following procedure is described in more detail in McKinney and Moelants (2004) and Moelants and McKinney (2004). Listeners were asked to tap to the beat of a series of musical excerpts. Responses were collected and their perceived tempo was calculated. For each excerpt, a distribution of perceived tempo was generated. A relatively simple form of perceived tempo was proposed for this contest: The two highest peaks in the perceived tempo distribution for each excerpt were taken, along with their respective heights (normalized to sum to 1.0) as the two tempo candidates for that particular excerpt. The height of a peak in the distribution is assumed to represent the perceptual salience of that tempo.
Perceptual tempo data collection for the GiantSteps dataset (Knees et al. 2015) was conducted in an online tapping experiment described in detail in Schreiber und Müller (2018). Just like the original McKinney/Moelants dataset, its annotations feature two tempi and a relative salience value. The dataset is publicly available for inspection, but must not be used for training.
References
- McKinney, M.F. and Moelants, D. (2004), Deviations from the resonance theory of tempo induction, Conference on Interdisciplinary Musicology, Graz. URL: http://www-gewi.uni-graz.at/staff/parncutt/cim04/CIM04_paper_pdf/McKinney_Moelants_CIM04_proceedings_t.pdf
- Moelants, D. and McKinney, M.F. (2004), Tempo perception and musical content: What makes a piece slow, fast, or temporally ambiguous? International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition, Evanston, IL. URL: http://icmpc8.umn.edu/proceedings/ICMPC8/PDF/AUTHOR/MP040237.PDF
- Schreiber, H. and Müller, M. (2018), A Crowdsourced Experiment for Tempo Estimation of Electronic Dance Music. In Proceedings of the 19th International Society for Music Information Retrieval Conference (ISMIR), Paris, France, Sept. 2018. URL: http://www.tagtraum.com/download/2018_schreiber_tempo_giantsteps.pdf
- Knees, P. et al. (2015), Two data sets for tempo estimation and key detection in electronic dance music annotated from user corrections. In Proceedings of the 16th International Society for Music Information Retrieval Conference (ISMIR), Málaga, Spain, October 2015. URL: http://www.mtg.upf.edu/system/files/publications/246_Paper.pdf
Evaluation of tempo extraction algorithms
Algorithms will process musical excerpts and return the following data: Two tempi in BPM (T1 and T2, where T1 is the slower of the two tempi). For a given algorithm, the performance, P, for each audio excerpt will be given by the following equation:
P = ST1 * TT1 + (1 - ST1) * TT2
where ST1 is the relative perceptual strength of T1 (given by groundtruth data, varies from 0 to 1.0), TT1 is the ability of the algorithm to identify T1 to within 8%, and TT2 is the ability of the algorithm to identify T2 to within 8%. No credit will be given for tempi other than T1 and T2.
The algorithm with the best average P-score will achieve the highest rank in the task.
Relevant Test Collections
We will use a collection of 160 musical exerpts for the evaluation procedure. 40 of the excerpts have been taken from one of McKinney/Moelants previous experiments (See McKinney/Moelants ICMPC paper above).
Excerpts were selected to provide:
- stable tempo within each excerpt
- a good distribution of tempi across excerpts
- a large variety of instrumentation and beat strengths (with and without percussion)
- a variation of musical styles, including many non-western styles
- the presence of non-binary meters (about 20% have a ternary element and there are a few examples with odd or changing meter).
We will provide 20 excerpts with ground truth data for participants to try/tune their algorithms before submission. The remaining 140 excerpts will be novel to all participants.
Practice Data
You can find it here:
https://www.music-ir.org/evaluation/MIREX/data/2006/beat/
User: beattrack Password: b34trx
https://www.music-ir.org/evaluation/MIREX/data/2006/tempo/
User: tempo Password: t3mp0
Data has been uploaded in both .tgz and .zip format.
Time and hardware limits
Due to the potentially high number of participants in this and other audio tasks, hard limits on the runtime of submissions will be imposed.
A hard limit of 8 hours will be imposed on analysis times. Submissions exceeding this limit may not receive a result.
Potential Participants
name / email